Macaroni Penguin


(Photo Gallery at bottom of page)


Macaroni Penguin Portrait Specific Name: Eudyptes chrysolophus
Pinguino Macaroni Gorfou Macaroni Goldschopfpinguin
Adult Height: 65-75cm
Adult Weight: 3.5-4kg (up to 6kg pre-moult)
Adult Flipper Length: 20cm
Estimated Population: Approx. 25 Million (incl. 11 Million breeding pairs)

Distribution:

The Macaroni Penguin is the most common of all penguins and has a wide geographical range across the subantarctic islands (see map). Some authors consider the Royal penguin to belong to the same species, although the general convention is to treat them separately. Macaronis and Royals can be visually distinguished, since the Macaroni usually has a dark chin. Small numbers of darker-chinned morphs of Royal Penguins can however be observed at Royal Penguin colonies. The Macaroni and Royal Penguins are the largest of the crested penguin species.



The highest concentrations of Macaroni Penguins are found on South Georgia, Crozet and Kerguelen Islands, and the Heard and McDonald Islands. The population on S. Georgia alone was about 2.8 million breeding pairs in the mid 1990s, with over 2.56 million of these on the Willis Islands at the NW tip of S. Georgia and another 90000 on neighbouring Bird Island (Trathan et al., 1998. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 169, p.263-275). The concentration of penguins at the NW tip can be explained by the proximity of the nutrient rich southern Antarctic Circumpolar Current front (sACCf). The Heard & McDonald Islands have 2 million breeding pairs (Woehler 1991. Intl. Council Bird Preserv. Tech. Publ. 11, p.263-277).

Macaroni Penguin Breeding Range Macaroni Penguin

Feeding:



South Georgia Population



Most studies on the foraging of Macaroni Penguins have been conducted at Bird Island in S. Georgia. A comparison of stomach contents of provisioning adults taken in Feb. 1986 and 1994, revealed a major shift in prey taken by Macaroni Penguins. In 1986, the diet was composed of 95% antarctic krill (Euphausia superba), 2.7% amphipods and 2.3% fish (mainly Krefftichthys anderssoni or icefish (Champsocephalus gunnari)), whereas in 1994 krill accounted for only 13.1% of the diet, with amphipods and fish making up 67.3 and 15%, respectively, and cephalopods making up 1% (Croxall et al., 1999. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 177, p.115-131). The dietary change correlated to a 4-fold lower krill biomass in 1994 and not only involved a shift in diet, but also caused a 50% reduction in meal size (brought ashore), whilst foraging trip length did not increase significantly. Breeding success was reduced by 10% but the fledging chicks were lighter and may have had reduced chances of survival. The Macaroni was nevertheless much more able to cope with the krill deficiency in 1994 than the Gentoo Penguins which suffered an almost complete breeding failure at nearby colonies, even though krill tends to form a smaller proportion of their diets. The greater comparative foraging range of Macaroni Penguins and resulting higher flexibility may explain this difference.



Studies on the diving of female Macaroni Penguins feeding largely on Antarctic Krill were performed during the brood period at S. Georgia and revealed that most dives were approximately V-shaped and reached maximum depths of about 115 meters (Croxall et al., 1993. J. Zool. Lond. 230, p.31-47). When the krill taken by Macaroni Penguins (or indeed Antarctic Fur Seals) in 1986 is compared with that caught in scientific fishing nets at the same location and time, it is found that the penguins preferentially feed on larger krill (mean length 56 mm) with a higher proportion of sexually active female krill in particular. These females are the largest krill available and this may account for the selectivity, together possibly with the greater ability of male krill, especially the more muscular and thus mobile mature males, to avoid capture (Reid et al., 1996. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 140, p.13-20).



Heard Population



Studies have also been performed on foraging ecology of chick-rearing Macaroni Penguins at 2 colonies on Heard Island in 1992 and 1993 (Green et al.,1998. Mar. Ornithol. 26, p.27-34). In 1992, average diet consisted of (by mass) the crustaceans T. gaudichaudii (37%) and E. vallentini (21.6%), and 41.4% fish (almost exclusively K. anderssoni, with an average length of 65 mm (in 1993)). As the breeding season progressed, the proportion of fish gradually increased as the level of E. vallentini dropped from 78% at the beginning of the observation period to 5% by its end. Foraging dives occurred almost exclusively during daylight hours and showed a more complex pattern than the previous studies. Looking only at dives of greater than 20 m, whilst some (57%) dive profiles were essentially V-shaped (round-bottomed), others were flat-bottomed (25.5%), and some were two-phased (17.5%), having two bottom-components separated by either a slight increase or decrease in depth. Females had an increased tendency towards shallow dives (<20m) or V-shaped dives, whilst males made less shallow dives and also had a slightly higher proportion of flat-bottomed dives. No clear correlation between time of day and dive depth was evident. Maximum dive depths of up to 163 m were recorded in the study, with maximum durations of just under 4 minutes. During the study period the birds foraged over a range of up to 300 km.



Foraging of chick-rearing Macaroni Penguins was studied in the 2003/04 season at the Capsize Beach colony on Heard Island by a combination of satellite tracking, depth logger and dietary analysis (Deagle et al., 2008. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 359, p.295-309). The penguins made foraging trips of a mean length and range of 23 hours and 47 km, respectively, during the guard period and 115 h and 169 km (max. 347 km) during the creche phase, although the latter figures are skewed upwards by a couple of male birds making exceptionally long (up to 12 day) trips. A number of patterns could be detected. Diving activity peaked around sunrise and sunset, presumably suggesting that prey is most accessible at these times and light still sufficient. Accordingly, no deep foraging dives (>10 m) were recorded during the hours of darkness. Further, foraging behaviour could be divided into 2 groups during the guard stage. The first involved the common pelagic diving in relatively deep waters, whereas a second group was found performing dives involving rapid ascent and descent and extended bottom times at relatively conserved and shallow depths, presumably signifying foraging at the sea floor (i.e. benthic diving). Trips performed by the second group had a mean length of only 6 h, whereas those of the first lasted on average 23.4 h. Benthic foraging has also been observed in Rockhopper Penguins. It was also found that those birds returning to the colony with significant proportions of fish in their stomach contents had generally been on longer trips involving deeper dives. Additionally, it was found that mean ascent and descent rates increased as dive depth increased from 0-35 m, with maximum descent rates reaching about 1.3m/sec and ascent rates reaching about 1.2m/sec during benthic dives, and about 1.1 and 1 m/sec, respectively in pelagic dives.

Further studies on foraging of penguins during the chick-rearing season are described in the section on reproduction.



Winter Foraging Behaviour



Long-term studies on diving behaviour are possible by deploying implantable data loggers. These are surgically introduced into the abdominal cavity of the penguin and recovered the next year when it returns to the colony. Such loggers are small and can record data for long periods as they avoid the power consumption required to ongoing data transmission. Macaroni penguins at Bird Island, S. Georgia were equipped with loggers that recorded dive depth, body attitude (upright or horizontal), heart rate and body temperature (Green et al., 2005. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 296, p.183-196). The study penguins were observed over 2 seasons from 2001-2003 and provided an immense amount of data which can only be summarized here. Dive duration was higher during the winter and males had significantly longer mean dive durations than females but dived less often, probably due to their larger body sizes. Less time per day was devoted to foraging during the winter, but during foraging bouts a greater amount of time was spent under water due to the longer dive lengths. This resulted in an average winter bottom time of over 1 min, at opposed to about 50 sec during the breeding season, with males spending slightly longer at the bottom than females. Mean descent rates were about 1.15 m/sec in the winter, as opposed to about 1.05 in the summer with lower decent rates resulting from the shallower diving during this period. Ascent rates averaged about 0.95 m/sec. Males generally ascended and descended marginally faster. When shallow dives (<10 m (29.3% of dives)) were excluded, mean dive depth was just over 40 m during the breeding season and averaged over 50 m during the winter season, although with significant interannual variation. Time devoted to diving activity correlated strongly to day length, especially during the winter. Foraging effort (i.e. time spent submerged per day) was highest in the run-up to the breeding season, presumably as penguins increased their efforts to reach an optimal weight of about 5 kg at the time they return to their colonies. The study also confirmed that the penguins did not return to land at all during the over 200 day winter foraging period. The location of the birds was not assessed.



Dive Angles During Searching and Foraging



An interesting question is why penguins do not dive vertically, since at first site it would appear most efficient to take the most direct route to and from the location where the prey is found. This question has been addressed in incubation phase female Macaroni Penguins on Kerguelen, to which data loggers were attached which were able to record body angles and acceleration both forward and up/down, the latter indicating flipper strokes (Sato et al., 2004. J. Exp. Biol. 207, p.4057-4065). The study revealed a number of aspects of diving behaviour. It was found that penguins descend by beating their flippers with a mean frequency of 2.3 Hz, forage at a slightly lower but more variable frequency at the bottom of a dive, and then start to ascend using a lower frequency before stopping flipper propulsion completely and gliding to the surface using their buoyancy alone. When prey was encountered on a dive it was found that the ascent angle was steeper and the descent angle of the following dive was also steep. This reduces horizontal movement and allows repeated visits to the same patch of prey. If no prey was encountered, the ascent took place at a shallow body angle, as did the following ascent. This increased horizontal movement presumably helps the penguin search for new patches of prey. Maximum angles of up to -80' were reached on decent and 70' on ascent (-90' is obviously a vertical dive). Mean angles were about -33 and 29', for descent and ascent, respectively. The body angles generally became particularly shallow during the last part of the ascent, possible because buoyancy is highest at this point, thus allowing the penguins to maintain vertical speed whilst increasing their horizontal motion merely by flipper directional control. It is possible that virtually all horizontal transiting is achieved by diving at shallow angles, since no active swimming was observed by birds at the surface.



Body Temperature During Diving



Studies involving implanted data loggers recording abdominal temperature have revealed that the abdominal temperatures fall quite rapidly once the penguins engage in a series of foraging dives. For example, a provisioning penguin monitored during the creche phase foraged for 8 hours and in that period its body temperature dropped gradually from just over 40'C pre-foraging to about 28'C (Green et al., 2002. Funct. Ecol. 16, p.671-681). Within the following half hour the temperature rapidly recovered to pre-foraging levels. The basal heart rate was around 100 beats/sec and regularly approached 300 whilst diving. More recent studies analysed abdominal temperature over a period of a year (Green et al., 2005. J. Exp. Biol. 208, p.2581-2593). The pre-moult trip mean temperature range was from about 29.5 to 39.5'C with daily averages lying around 36'C. During the winter trip the temperature range was marginally smaller and the mean daily average was at about 35'C for females and 36'C for the larger males. Males generally had higher mean, max. and minimum temperatures, with the differences lying within the range of about 0.5-2'C. Seasonal fluctuations were also observed which may be accounted for by changes in body condition and/or water temperature in the foraging areas.

The drop in abdominal temperature may be partially accounted for by the ingestion of cold food and sea-water whilst foraging and by reduced insulation and colder water temperatures at depth. Penguins keep warm by a combination of their plumage (with the entrapped air therein) and internal fat deposits. When on land, the feathers may be erected and plumage may account for 80% of insulation, yet this may be reduced to 10% in water since the feathers lie close to the body to remain watertight and the remaining air is largely lost or compressed during diving (Barre and Roussel 1986. Am. J. Physiol. 251, p.R456-R462).



Foraging Range (Summer)



The foraging range of Macaroni Penguins has been studied by satellite tracking at Bird Island, S. Georgia, during the breeding season (Barlow and Croxall 2002. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 232, p.291-304). On the long trips (10-26 days) during the incubation phase, birds travelled largely in a NW direction to the Maurice Ewing Bank, many of the birds reaching as far as the Polar Frontal Zone (PFZ) where Myctophid and Cephalopod concentrations are considered high. Birds travelled a horizontal distance of about 4.2 km/h on average with no significant differences between travel time at day and night, and the males trips were longer in mean duration (M:18d; F:12d), distance travelled (M:1827 km; F:1236 km) and maximum range (M:572 km; F:376 km). It should be noted that during the study, Krill availability was low, which may have influenced foraging behaviour. During the guard/creche phase, 80% of trips lasted less than 2 days and involved slower horizontal travel and mean ranges of only 62 km with most penguins remaining over the continental shelf around S. Georgia.

Further tracking studies of female birds during the brood/guard phase at several sites on S. Georgia revealed interesting data about the respective foraging locations of birds from different colonies (Trathan et al., 2006. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 323, p.239-251). Birds tended to leave their colonies on relatively constant bearings, usually heading directly away from the coast line and towards the shelf-break zone. Thus the bearings during short foraging trips apparently largely depend on colony position. A similar concentration of foraging trip bearings was reported in a study on chick-rearing penguins foraging at Heard Island (Deagle et al., 2008. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 359, p.295-309).

As of 2008, no study had been published revealing the location of Macaroni penguins during the winter foraging period.


Reproduction:



Nest and Partner Selection



Macaroni Penguins nest in large dense colonies often located on sloping ground above small cliffs. Compared to Rockhopper Penguins, with which they may establish mixed colonies in places, Macaronis nest on slightly more open ground, less often seeking the immediate shelter of rocks. Colonies are usually free of vegetation due to the wear on the vegetation imposed by the nesting penguins, although the penguins will also nest in surrounding areas of tussock grass. The colony is usually connected to the sea by one main walkway which may include extremely steep and rocky sections which the penguins are adept at mastering. The use of a single walkway facilitates monitoring of the transit of birds to and from colonies since it allows the use of automated gateways which detect passage of birds implanted with tiny passive transponders (C. Green et al., 2006. Polar Biol. 29(12), p.1993-1010). Penguins may spend a significant time transiting from the coast to their colony, for example 30 min each way at the Fairy Point colony, Bird Island, S. Georgia (Barlow and Croxall 2002. Ibis 144, p.248-258).

Macaroni Penguin Colony, Elsehul, South Georgia Elsehul Macaroni Penguin Colony South Georgia Elsehul Macaroni Penguin Colony

Macaroni Penguin Colony, Elsehul, S. Georgia

Elsehul Macaroni Colony, Walkway

Elsehul Macaroni Colony. Inset: Penguins on walkway

The breeding season begins as birds return to their colonies after their extended winter foraging trips. Mass of penguins on arrival may be a critical determinant of breeding success (Crawford et al., 2007. Afr. J. Mar. Sci. 30(1), p.185-188; Raya Rey et al., 2007. Ibis 149, p.826-835). Male birds return first to establish nest sites, whereas females return about a week later. Nests are relatively simply and usually consist of little more than a slight hollow or a small pile of pebbles which may be lined with small amounts of plant material. The male is primarily responsible for collecting any nesting material, whilst the female tends to make adjustments during the egg-laying phase.

Fidelity to nest site and partners has been studied from 1986 to 1990 at Bird Island, South Georgia (Williams and Rodwell 1992. Condor 94, p.636-645). Nest site fidelity ranged from 69-87%, with mate fidelity between 74 and 78%. Separation was observed in 4-13% of cases, with non-return (probable death) of at least one of the mates accounting for the remainder (up to 19%). Successful breeders were less likely to change mate, yet subsequent breeding success was not higher in repeat breeders. It appears that males consider maintenance of nest site of primary importance. Males are reluctant to leave their nest sites to find a new partner and about 40% of males do not breed for at least 1 year following separation. However, females will find a new mate, usually within a few meters of the old nest site, in 94% of cases. Also, after separation in the case when both birds are alive, all males studies remained at the nest site, whilst the females established themselves at nearby nests. The high partner fidelity observed is probably accounted for by the high nest fidelity, which provides a focal point for re-pairing, together with the highly synchronized arrival of Macaroni Penguins at the breeding site, which allows little time for partner change.

Interestingly, about half of those birds whos partner did not return to moult already established a new partner during the moult period. Of these new partners, about half were actually mated with in the subsequent season. Hence, the moult appears to be an important period for establishing new pair bonds.



Courtship and Copulation



Courtship behaviour involves advertising such as bowing or the ecstatic display, in which eudyptid penguins point their heads skyward, extend their flippers and make a series of cries with wide open bills as they swing their heads from side to side. As courtship progresses, the pair may perform mutual displaying where both partners bow, quiver and perform ecstatic displays synchronously.

Copulation occurs at the nest site and may be instigated by either partner. The female may indicate willingness by squatting on the nest or the male by beating the female with one or both of his flippers. The male huddles up to the female, nibbles her neck gently and pats her with one of his flippers. The male then eventually mounts his partner who lies in a position with the head raised, and then edges backwards approaching the cloaca whilst using his flippers to pat the female's flanks. Both partners bring their cloaca towards each other so that contact can be made. The male briefly holds his position, stabilized by his flippers and usually also by the female who holds his neck with her bill. The male finally dismounts and adopts a subdued hunched posture. Both birds may proceed to preen or rest for some time following copulation.



Laying / Brood Reduction Mechanism



First eggs are usually laid by the end of November in S. Georgia. This time may fluctuate slightly between years or site. Two eggs are laid over a period of about 4 days, a smaller (A) egg, followed by the larger (B) egg. Egg composition studies revealed that the yolk of the (A) egg is proportionally larger (although smaller in absolute terms) than that of the (B) egg (30 v 24%), with the albumen (white) being accordingly smaller (57 v 63%), and the egg shells making up the rest (12 v 13%) (Gwynn 1993. Emu 93, p.290-291). It should be noted that it has been suggested by one author that the (A) egg is simply smaller since it develops during a period when yoke deposition is below maximum (Williams 1990. Oikos 59(3), p.349-354).



All crested penguin species display egg dimorphism and associated brood reduction mechanisms, favouring the second and larger (B) egg. There are however significant differences in the extent of dimorphism and the kind of brood reduction behaviour displayed. Macaroni, Royal and Erect-Crested Penguins show the most extreme form of brood reduction and appear to actively remove the first laid (A) egg from the nest, whilst the Rockhopper, Fiordland Crested and Snares Penguins do not actively remove the (A) egg and may in rare cases even raise 2 chicks. The Macaroni Penguins eggs are the second most dimorphic of those found in the Eudyptid Penguins, only surpassed by those of the Erect-Crested Penguin. On Bird Island, S. Georgia, the first (A) egg and second (B) egg having respective mean masses of 94 and 145 g, and a mean difference in size of 60% (although varying from 20% to 90% in different clutches) during the 1998 breeding season (Barlow 2001. Antarctic Sci. 13(3), p.286-287). An earlier study at Marion Island had found the mean size difference to be about 62% with hardly any variation, whilst the sizes of both eggs compared from one clutch to another fluctuated by up to 73%, as opposed to about 40% in the Barlow study (Williams 1990. Oikos 59(3), p.349-354).



Loss of the (A) egg occurred in 54% of nests prior to laying of the (B) egg during a study of Macaroni Penguins on Marion Island (Williams 1980. Auk 97, p.754-759). Similar figures were found in a later study which showed peak loss on the day before laying of the (B) egg and on the same day. All (A) eggs were lost by the end of the day on which the (B) egg was laid (Barlow 2001. Antarctic Sci. 13(3), p.286-287). Studies on the similar Royal Penguin show that eggs are removed from the nest by use of the bill, or are scraped out of the nest with the feet. This usually coincided with a period of increased parental nest-building behaviour preceding laying of the second egg (Cassady St Clair et al., 1995. Animal Behaviour 50, p.1177-1185). The reasons for this extreme form of brood reduction involving expulsion of the in principle viable (A) egg at such an early stage are not clear. Egg expulsion apparently unnecessarily limits the (A) egg insurance value to the 4 days preceding laying of the (B) egg. It is possible that the (A) egg primarily serves as an insurance against the failure to lay a second (B) egg. In studies where (A) eggs were artificially substituted for (B) eggs in some nests, and subsequently naturally incubated until hatching, it was found that (A) chicks had on average 55% of the mass of (B) chicks at hatching (Williams 1990. Oikos 59(3), p.349-354). By fledging, (A) and (B) chicks had the same sizes, and mass was similar or only marginally lower in (A) chicks. Hence, (A) eggs evidently have a similar potential to (B) eggs.



Experiments have shown that if a second egg (transferred from another nest) is added to the nest shortly after laying of the (A) egg, loss of the (A) egg is not accelerated. In fact, both the (A) egg and transferred egg were removed from the nest as usual within 24 hours of laying the (B) egg (Barlow 2001. Antarctic Sci. 13(3), p.286-287). In the same study it was shown that a transferred egg added to the nest after laying of the (B) egg was retained for an average of 6 days and was always incubated in the anterior position, which is preferentially taken by the smaller (A) egg in other Eudyptid Penguin species that incubate 2 eggs. Hence, Macaroni Penguins will not rapidly eject a second egg from the nest after the (B) egg has been laid. The fact that two eggs can clearly be incubated by Macaroni Penguins makes it even more mystifying why the (A) egg is lost so early.



Incubation



Incubation duties are divided into two main shifts, which are preceded by a phase where both parents perform a number of short alternating foraging trips. When a partner returns to the nest, the partners usually greet each other. This can involve loud trumpeting calls which may be performed with the head pointing upwards and the flippers flapping up and down, and/or may include some head-swinging as in the ecstatic display. If nest relief occurs, the partners adopt a relatively hunched posture and the relieving bird shuffles slowly onto the nest, shifts from foot to foot a couple of times and then gently settles onto the nest. When the main incubation shifts start, the male leaves to forage and regain weight whilst the female remains on the nest. This period lasts for about 10-20 days. The remainder of the incubation duties are then largely left to the male, whilst the female forages (Williams and Croxall 1991. J. Zool. (London) 223(2), p. 189-202). During the male incubation shift, females (if foraging locally) may sporadically return to the nest and perform brief incubation shifts (mean 10 h) whilst the male stands nearby (Haftorn 1986. Polar Res. 4 n.s. p.33-45). Interestingly, incubating males were observed to spend about 43% of their incubation duties in an upright position (including 3% of time preening, displaying, shifting the egg or turning on nest) and 57% in the prone (lying) position. The proportion of time spent upright is exceptionally high for an incubating penguin. It appears that it is possible to effectively incubate a single egg clutch effectively without adopting a prone posture, since egg temperature measurements revealed temperatures of over 37'C in the incubating birds.



Brood / Guard Phase



As in other Eudyptid penguins, after hatching of the egg, the male Macaroni Penguin remains on the nest and has sole responsibility for guarding the chick, whilst the female provisions the chick by making short 1-2 day foraging trips. Chicks hatch with a thin coat of down (protoptile plumage) which allows efficient heat transfer from the brooding parent. A thicker down (mesoptyl plumage) is developed within 2-3 weeks. Young chicks are brooded under the parent, but rapidly become too large to fit under it. Large chicks lie partially covered (usually with head and shoulders covered) by the parent or lean closely against it. The mesoptyl down is crucial for thermoemancipation of the chicks, and is a prerequisite for the transition from the guard to the creche phase which usually occurs after about 20-23 days at Bird Island. Once the chick is in the creche phase, both parents provision the chick. Creching is thought to provide some protection against predation. When parents return, the chicks need to be recognized so that they can be fed.



Creche Phase / Acoustic Parent-Chick Recognition



During the creche phase, the chick no longer remains at the nest and is usually found about 3-5 m away from it in a small group of chicks called a creche. Adult-chick recognition now relies on acoustic signals. When arriving at the nesting site, adults make several about 1.75 sec long calls, made up of a long 1st part (syllable), followed by a second part of similar length consisting of a series of 5-30 (av. 12.84) shorter syllables (Searby et al., 2004. Animal Behav. 67, p.615-625) of which the first are repetitive short syllables, with the last 2 syllables being slightly longer. The pattern (or tempo) of the syllables, together with the harmonic content, make the signal distinctive, i.e. Macaroni Penguins have a double vocal signature. The call is fundamentally similar to, but significantly longer than that of the Rockhopper Penguin (0.45 sec), and the individual syllables are more distinctive in the Macaroni penguin (Searby and Jouventin 2005. J. Avian Biol. 36, p.449-460).



Both of these Eudyptid penguin calls are less complex than the double vocal signature of the Aptenodytes penguins (i.e. Emperor, King), but more complex than the solely harmonic system in Pygoscelis penguins (e.g. Adelie, Gentoo). The higher complexity of the call in non-nesting penguin species is accounted for by the fact that the acoustic signal does not only have to distinguish between birds at a particular location (i.e. nest and immediate surroundings) within a colony, but has to be able to discriminate essentially all birds in a colony without a supplementing positional signal. Playback experiments of Macaroni parent calls were able to stimulate chicks to return to the nest in 39% of cases, whereas a parental call was responded to in 99% of cases. This confirms that a visual stimulus of a parent at the nest is also important. Further, by modifying calls, it could be shown that the second part of the call carries most recognition information, especially encoded by the duration of the repetitive syllables. On hearing a parental call, chicks call and approach the parent, after which feeding usually commences within less than a minute. Parents recognize their own chicks acoustically and peck other chicks that approach them in the hope of being fed.



Provisioning of Chicks



Based on studies using implanted data loggers which were able to record heart rate and dive depth for Penguins over a full year, it was calculated that only about 15% of prey consumed during the chick-rearing phase was actually provided to chicks by both male and female birds (Green et al., 2007. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 346, p.299-312). This could be broken down into brood phase (females provide 22.3% of food to chick) and creche phase (female 16.7% and males 10.2%). On an average foraging trip, most food caught near the beginning of the trip is digested by the adult. Food caught towards the end of the trip is brought ashore in a relatively undigested state (it is thought that penguins can downregulate their digestive activity) and fed to the chick as necessary, with the remainder then being digested by the parent on land. In the study, returning parents were thought to have only about 36% (female) and 42% (male) of their daily catch in their stomachs, whilst the rest had already been digested. Of this, the relative proportions digested by chick or parent vary depending on the stage in the chicks development. As the chick grows, its demands initially increase rapidly until levelling off and even sometimes dropping slightly shortly before fledging. Hence, the female needs to provide the chick with a progressively higher proportion of food (eventually nearly all) until the male joins her in foraging at the beginning of the creche phase. Females tend to combine numerous short trips with a number of longer ones during the creche phase and arrival and departure times become more erratic than during the brood phase, where they are largely concentrated at dawn and dusk (C. Green et al., 2006. Polar Biol. 29(12), p.1003-1010). This pattern had been observed previously and it has been suggested that the longer trips serve to allocate resources to the parent (Barlow and Croxall, 2002. Ibis 144, p.248-258).



Studies on provisioning behaviour at Bird Island, S. Georgia in the 1998-2000 breeding seasons detailed the relative contributions of male and female birds and also chick development (Barlow and Croxall 2002. Ibis 144, p.248-258). On average, females provided 44 meals to the chick (17 during the 23 day guard period, 27 during the 37 day creche period) and males 19 (all during creche). Hence, even during the creche period, females accounted for 60% of meals, and in some individual cases males hardly fed the chick at all. Birds tended to leave the colony at dawn and return over a broader time-period leading up to dusk, with trips lasting 16 h on average. Birds spend an average of about 14 h on land between trips, males tending to stay about 2 hours longer. A minor peak in trip length records was also found at 42 h, corresponding to largely female birds spending a night out at sea and returning the next evening. Over the 3 study seasons, males provided approx. 0.5 meals/day and females 0.67. The male starts the creche period at a lower weight than the female and thus needs to focus more on its own condition. Male foraging rate is correlated with the chicks rate of growth and fledging mass. Females appear to perform a mixture of short and long trips, and it has been suggested that this may serve to feed the chick and maintain own condition, respectively. Average meals were about 300 g with significant interseasonal fluctuations presumably resulting from fluctuations in prey availability. For example, meals during the creche phase averaged 191 g in 1999 (when provisioning was also less frequent), but 391 g, in 2000. Irrespective of this massive difference in food provided to the chicks, there were only small differences in growth rates between years. Chick growth is nearly linear for the first 40-50 days, after which a mean weight in the region of 3 kg is reached, and then becomes more erratic shortly before fledging. Creching occurred at about 22 days at a mass of between 1-1.7 kg (mean approx. 1450 g). Mean fledging mass was 3.3 kg in 2000 and only 2806 kg in 1999, when food was more scarce. Fledging usually takes place after about 60 days on Bird Island. If the male does not contribute meals during the creche phase, chicks may still survive to fledging but are likely to only fledge at a mass of about 2400 g. The level of male contribution thus largely determines the mass at fledging, which is probably critical for survival during the first year (Barlow and Croxall 2002. Ibis 144, p.248-258). In the study, breeding success varied from 45.3% in 1998 to 58.9% in 1999 (even though chicks received least food in this year). This is largely due to the failure of 42.2% of incubated eggs to hatch in 1998, possibly due to poor female body condition upon arrival at the colony in this season. On average over the 3 years, 28% of nests failed due to hatching failure, 22% failed during the guard phase and only 4.5% during the creche period.



Chick food consumption totals approx. 17 kg between hatching and fledging. When enough food is available, the chick consumes about 110 g/day (i.e. equivalent to its own mass) shortly after hatching. This gradually increases and ideally peaks at about 400 g/day after about 30 days before then gradually declining again to about 180 g/day just before fledging. The mean daily time spent submerged for parents follows a similar pattern (Green et al., 2007. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 346, p.299-312). Males were found to be more efficient foragers, extracting 2.84 g (as opposed to 2.28 g for females) from their environment for every minute spent submerged foraging. Only about 15% of prey caught during the chick rearing period is actually fed to the chick, meaning that total parental consumption is about 110 kg during this period. Daily prey consumption could reach as high as 48% of body mass in males and 41% in females.

The data from Bird Island is certainly not representative for all years or study sites, and indeed appears to represent a situation with relatively good foraging conditions. In a study at Marion Island, chicks were observed fledging at an average of 2.3 kg at an age of 70 days, due to lower food availability (Brown 1987. Polar Biol. 8, p.95-102). Also, a study on chick-rearing Macaroni Penguins at Heard Island, Capsize Beach Colony, in 2003-2004 revealed average meal sizes of only 71 g and on average longer trips and thus less frequent meals.



Fledging



Macaroni Penguins fledge after approx. 60 days at Bird Island and have usually attained a mass of about 3 kg by this time. Earlier studies at Marion Island revealed fledging after 70 days at a lower mean weight of 2.3 kg (Brown 1987. Polar Biol. 8, p.95-102). The water-proof juvenile plumage develops shortly before fledging and lacks the distinctive crest of the adult birds. Juvenile penguins have a small crest at each side of the head, brown (as opposed to red) eyes and greyish-brown (as opposed to orange) bills.

Fledglings spend their first year at sea after which they return to land to moult into their adult plumage. Macaroni Penguins usually only start to breed at an age of about 5 years.



Post-Breeding Moult



After the chicks have fledged, adult birds go on extended foraging trips for at least 2 weeks to gain weight before the annual moult. During this period, penguins may accumulate significant reserves of lipids and proteins and thereby increase in mass by 50%, whilst at the same time the new feathers start to grow under the skin. Birds may arrive on land with from about 20-40% of the length of the new feathers already grown. The moult is necessary, since feathers suffer wear and need to be occasionally replaced to ensure that the penguins maintain a water-proof and insulating plumage. During the moult, penguins are relatively sedentary and remain ashore fasting. The moult has long been considered highly energetically demanding, but a detailed study on the energetics of moulting female Macaroni Penguins on S. Georgia have shown that energy expenditure may not be higher than during the breeding season (Green et al., 2004. J. Avian Biol. 35, p.153-161). In this study, the average length of the moult was 22.3 days, although based on other studies, the moult may take anything from 13-34 days. As the moult progressed, body mass loss was more or less linear in absolute terms, whilst the mean heart rate (measured using an implanted logger) increased until peaking after the moult was 60-70% complete, after which it slightly dropped again. A similar pattern was observed for the abdominal temperature. At this stage, metabolic activity is elevated to compensate for surface heat loss. This is due to the almost complete loss of the old plumage, the high level of vascularization of the skin, and the fact that only about 50% of the length of the new feathers have emerged.

After completion of the moult, the penguins go on their long winter foraging trips during which they do not return to land until the start of the next breeding season.

Macaroni Penguin Incubating in Prone Position Macaroni Penguin with Chick Royal Penguin Path to Colony

Macaroni Penguin Incubating in Prone Position

Macaroni Penguin with Chick

Macaroni Penguin Inspecting Chick

Macaroni Penguin with Chick Macaroni Penguin with Chick Macaroni Penguin with Chick

Macaroni Penguin with Chick

Macaroni Penguin with Chick

Macaroni Penguin with Chick

Macaroni Penguin Chick Macaroni Penguin with Chick Macaroni Penguin Chick

Macaroni Penguin Chick

Macaroni Penguin with Chick

Macaroni Penguin Chick



General Behaviour:

Macaroni Penguin behaviour is largely similar to that of other penguins belonging to the Eudyptid genus. A number of displays are performed, a few of which are described here, or insofar as they relate directly to courtship, in the section above.

Macaroni Penguins are quite aggressive and will peck at other penguins or people moving close to their nests. When facing an adversary, they may inflate their cheeks slightly, bob their heads from side to side and/or up and down, often accompanying this with raising of flippers or with harsh cries. Opponents may be jabbed at by rapid extension of the neck with the bill open, ready to peck. If fighting results, adversaries attempt to try to grab each other at the back of the neck and beat each other with their flippers. Retreating birds are often chased and grabbed and pecked into the hind quarters with the bill. Males tend to be more aggressive than females, especially during the early stages of the breeding season as nest sites and partnerships are established. Aggression levels are lower during the incubation / guard phases so whilst it is possible that evolution has resulted in the male taking over the guard stage due to its higher aggressiveness, it would not seem that a particularly high level of aggression is favoured at this stage.

Much time on land is devoted to cleaning and rearranging the plumage. Birds may preen themselves or perform mutual preening (allopreening). The latter not only serves to maintain the condition of the plumage and to remove parasites, but also is thought to play a role in reinforcing pair bonds. Penguins may briefly venture to the sea in order to bathe and remove dirt and parasites from their bodies. Bathing penguins often twist and turn rapidly in the water and also may preen themselves whilst at sea. This behaviour is usually observed very close to shore.

When penguins are not busy foraging or maintaining their plumage, they spend most of their time resting in a standing or lying posture.

Macaroni Penguin Preening at Nest Macaroni Penguin Preening Flipper

Macaroni Penguin Preening by Nest

Macaroni Penguin Preening Flipper

Displaying Macaroni Penguin Resting Macaroni Penguin Preening Macaroni Penguin at Nest

Displaying Macaroni Penguin

Resting Macaroni Penguin

Preening Macaroni Penguin at Nest

Macaroni Penguin Preening Chick Macaroni Penguins Allopreening Macaroni Penguins Allopreening

Macaroni Penguin Preening Chick

Macaroni Penguins Allopreening

Macaroni Penguins Allopreening


Threats:

One has to distinguish between threats to individual penguins and to populations as a whole. Individual penguins may be hunted by Leopard Seals and also by Orcas or Fur Seals, numbers of which have been rising dramatically in recent years. Eggs suffer significant levels of avian predation, especially from Skuas, but also from Sheathbills and Gulls. The chicks are larger and faster-growing compared to those of Rockhoppers (for example) and thus only suffer predation from Skuas when they are relatively young (Williams 1980. Auk 97, p.754-759).



Whilst dietary studies have shown the ability of Macaroni Penguins to compensate for reductions in availability of certain prey species, long term declines are likely to have an impact on population levels since there are clearly thresholds below which the population declines. The effect of climate change and commercial fisheries activities are of particular concern and the effects of reduced overall availability and increased prey patchiness have been modelled (Cresswell et al., 2008. Polar Biol. 31, p.641-649). The Krill population at S. Georgia is largely sustained by oceanic currents carrying Krill from breeding grounds around the Antarctic Peninsula. Juvenile Krill are highly dependent on ice-cover for food and shelter and the reduced amounts of sea-ice resulting from global warming trends and likely to have a long-term negative effect on Krill levels. Further, in El Nino Southern Oscillation (ENSO) years, the southern Antarctic Circumpolar Current front (sACCf) which carries the Krill to S. Georgia appears to be weakened, resulting in reduced Krill availability. There is a risk that general climatic trends may also affect these currents. Commercial Krill fisheries are presently increasing and whilst well below the limits imposed by the CCAMLR at present, this may not remain the case for long. The fisheries focus on areas of high Krill density, much like the penguins, so there is clearly a high risk of direct competition. Further, in S. Georgia, the Macaroni Penguins may be suffering increased competition for their preferred prey (krill) from the Antarctic Fur Seal population which has increased rapidly from 400000 in 1972 to over 3 million by 2001 (Barlow et al., 2002). These species both preferentially populate the NW tip of the Islands and studies on populations on Bird Island have shown that their foraging ranges overlap and that they feed on krill of similar size and in similar depths, but that the seals tend to concentrate foraging activity further from land than penguins provisioning chicks. Nevertheless, it seems that increased krill consumption by the seals is reducing krill availability for the penguins.



Especially those populations nesting on isolated islands are not in a position to shift their breeding sites in response to climatic trends. If significant prey populations shift away from the breeding site, foraging parents will be unable to sufficiently provision their chicks.


Where To See:

Not many Macaroni penguin colonies are accessible to visitors. A small number of Macaroni penguins breed on the Falklands, yet these generally breed within Rockhopper Penguin colonies and may thus not be easy to find, and if not at the periphery of the colony difficult to observe. Macaroni Penguins may also be seen on Cruises including S. Georgia on their itineraries, yet the most frequently visited colony at Cooper Bay (7700 breeding pairs in 1986) has strict regulations on the number of visitors and the main colony cannot generally be accessed as birds nest in the surrounding tussock grass areas. The colony at Elsehul (2600 breeding pairs in 1986) can be seen from boats visiting this harbour, but pedestrian access is not generally granted.

Cooper Bay Macaroni Colony Cooper Bay Macaroni Penguin Colony

Cooper Bay, S. Georgia

Cooper Bay Macaroni Colony

Macaroni Penguins, Elsehul

Penguins near Elsehul Macaroni Colony, Viewed from Zodiac.



Photo Gallery:



Macaroni Penguins Macaroni Penguins Macaroni Penguin with Chick

Macaroni Penguins Macaroni Penguin Royal Penguin

Macaroni Penguin with Chick Macaroni Penguin with Chick



Penguin Index



Photovolcanica Full Index